Tuesday, 30 March 2010
Public order policing (again)
At the very least this is bad PR on the behalf of the police. No-one likes a killjoy. But I'm inclined to agree with George Monbiot and others and say that actions like this are fundamentally undemocratic. People should have a right to freely gather, even if the end is simply (gasp!) to enjoy themselves. It's the job of the police to facilitate this, not stop it happening. The fact that 73 people out of c.20,000 were arrested last year is neither here nor there. In any large gathering you're going to get people misbehaving - look at football matches, for example - but that doesn't mean the gathering should be prevented from occurring.
Friday, 26 March 2010
A peculiarly British obsession
This is so strong a theme that it appears that press and politicians follow public opinion on this issue, in contrast to their more usual stance of assuming people think a certain way in the face of evidence to the contrary. Think for example about the divergence between how punitive people are thought to be and how punitive they actually are (see here for an interesting discussion of some of the issues). The public obsession with this type of policing is so strong that police officer's have been heard to complain that some people would like a constable standing permanently on their doorstep - keeping the world at bay, presumably.
In this context it's hardly surprising that the Home Office have apparently tried to inflate the proportion of time neighbourhood teams spend 'on the beat'. One issue is the usual one of government setting itself targets (here, the policing pledge) and then having to wangle the figures, to a greater or lesser extent, to meet them. Wangle more, here, I think, because I suspect 'attending neighbourhood meetings' is not what most people would mean by 'on the beat' - and that's before the ASA's concerns about the 80 per cent claim appearing to apply to all 140,000 thousand officers kicks in.
But perhaps another issue is more fundamental. Is it really an efficient use of resources to have sworn officers wandering around the streets waiting for something to happen, or could their time be put to better use? While some beat patrolling will always be a good idea, especially that done by PCSOs, should the police be the first and only institution involved in providing visible agents of social control in local areas? That they pretty much are says a lot about the gradual removal from public space of others who used to do something similar - park wardens, bus conductors and so on - and is probably an example of the gradual colonisation of that space by the criminal law, at the expense of other ways of maintaining order.
One argument is of course that patrolling police make people feel better. And they probably do, to some extent. But is this enough in itself to warrant such expensive activity? Are there other benefits to beat patrols - do they have any deterrent effect, for example?Actually, they might, if limited to crime hotspots, although you kind of get the impression that such targeting would go against the spirit of the policing pledge. What about deterrence more widely? Has anyone done some kind of cost benefit analysis, or is beat patrol considered to be a good in and of itself? If it is, for example in terms of public confidence, how long do the effects last - are they effervescent, only lasting for a few minutes, or longer lived? Do they apply to all people, or are some scared or annoyed by a very visible police presence?
These are all really interesting questions, and I certainly don't think we have answers to them all yet. Which makes it all the more bizarre that the Home Office should make itself a hostage to targets which may not actually be that useful and which in any case it has to fudge in order to achieve. And then undermine public confidence in crime statistics again by playing a bit fast and loose with the figures.
Thursday, 25 March 2010
Watching them watching you watching them
Only a few years ago this case, if it had gone to court at all, would have been decided on contradictory personal accounts and, perhaps, police video footage (and further back in time, personal accounts only). In either case a conviction would probably be the most likely outcome; that's a guess, but it doesn't seem unreasonable.
The fact that in addition to all the film the police hold there are now thousands of hours of footage of demonstrations and other situations routinely uploaded to Youtube - and definitively into the public domain - seems to mark a sea-change in what police and protesters can expect to get away with. This looks like a situation that police are going to have to learn to live with, just as people involved in demonstrations have over the last 10-15 years.
Wednesday, 24 March 2010
Monday, 22 March 2010
"Ugly criminals"?
"Being very attractive reduces a young adult’s propensity for criminal activity and being unattractive increases it. Being very attractive is also positively associated with wages and with adult vocabulary test scores, which implies that beauty may have an impact on human capital formation. The results suggest that a labour market penalty provides a direct incentive for unattractive individuals toward criminal activity. The level of beauty in high school is associated with criminal propensity seven to eight years later, which seems to be due to the impact of beauty in high school on human capital formation, although this avenue seems to be effective for females only" (Mocan and Tekin 2010).
I want to use this post to discuss a recent paper that appeared in The Review of Economics and Statistics (full version here if you can access it, otherwise the abstract above will have to do). Initially because I though the idea was so mad it was worth a few comments, but having re-read the paper more now because I think it throws up some very interesting points beyond its specific empirical content.
The article is based on analysis of the US National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (n=15,338, which is relevant). It uses respondent’s ‘beauty’ (controlling for a very wide range of other variables) to predict self-reported criminal behaviour. It’s worth noting that the mechanisms the paper suggests for attractiveness affecting criminal activity are all social – it essentially claims that the less attractive are more likely to commit crime (and the more attractive less likely) because they are treated differently by those around them, in terms of access to the jobs market, treatment at school, and so forth (hence for example the lower vocabulary scores mentioned above - it's not that 'ugly' people are thick, its that they get treated differently at school and this inhibits their learning process).
Methods and stats
First, let’s get the methods/statistics out of the way. With two provisos they are actually quite convincing. It really does seem that the physical attractiveness of females (although to a lesser extent males – see below) is associated with self-reported criminal activity, in the ways suggested in the abstract above.
The two provisos are quite important, though. Firstly, how is physical attractiveness measured? It turns out by the interviewers in the survey. There is something immediately almost repellent about this, but the authors present some evidence that such ratings are fairly robust, and I don’t want to get bogged down in this point, so let’s just take it as read for now that they do have a somewhat accurate measure of the survey respondent’s physical attractiveness.
The second proviso is even more important. Namely, that the effect sizes they identify are very small. For example, “Being a very attractive female reduces the propensity to damage property by 1.1 percentage points” (controlling for personal and family characteristics). This effect is statistically significant, because of the very large sample size. But is it substantively meaningful? Does it make any sense to concentrate on such a small effect? The authors clearly think so, and up to a point I agree with them – the social world is a complicated place, and the unique association between any one factor (e.g. physical attractiveness) and another (e.g. propensity to vandalise things) is almost bound to be small. If there is a statistically significant association, robust to all the control variables that seem relevant, it probably does mean something.
But on the other hand, there must come a point where an association, although significant, is so small that it really can’t have much meaning – certainly if we want to change the world in some way. A one percentage point difference is interesting in the context of this paper, but would be so if, for example, some sort of policy intervention was being planned? I think we as social scientists do not pay enough attention to the pay off between statistical significance and effect size, and this paper butts right up against that point.
One other point on the specific empirical content – as noted the effect of ‘attractiveness’ on crime appear much more consistent for women than for men. I’m not too sure what to make of this, and neither I think are the authors. While it might be consistent with their theoretical framework (since it appears physical attractiveness as rated by others varies by more among women than among men, it should therefore have a bigger effect), it’s unclear how this fits with ‘explaining crime’, given the well known gender disproportionality in criminal activity.
Three more substantive points
There are (at least) three more substantive points I’d like to make about the paper, again because these seem to me to be relevant far beyond its specific content (there are more but these particularly strike me).
First is the rational choice theory model of criminal behaviour used in the paper. It essentially takes as a given the idea that people commit crimes when there is motive (reward) to do so, there is an opportunity, and the rewards outweigh the potential costs. Yet much recent work on why people commit crime would stress the importance of morals and normative values – to the extent that most people do not commit crime because they do not even see it as an option, even in situations where they would gain from doing so.
Now, this may not make much difference in terms of this specific paper, because we would expect normative orientations toward the law to be evenly distributed across ‘attractive’ and ‘unattractive’ people and thus not affect the results. But taking such a narrowly instrumentalist approach toward decisions to commit crime does I think in the wider context run the risk of over-simplifying what seems certain to be a very complex process.
More serious for the specific claims of the paper is its very one-sided view of what crime is. The types of crimes covered by the analysis are ‘damage’ (vandalism), burglary, robbery, theft, assault and selling drugs; essentially, ‘crimes of the poor’. What about white collar crime? Tax evasion? Fraud? Corporate crime? At the very least the omission of a wider range of criminal behaviour limits the claims that can be made by the paper. But worse, it may be that being physical attractive, and thus preferred in the job market to others, opens up greater opportunity to engage in white collar crime (by increasing both opportunity and level of potential gain). So it could be in these cases physical attractiveness is linked to higher rates of self-reported offending. Of course, such a impoverished of crime is not unique to this paper – it seems to bedevil many economic and similar approaches to criminological matters.
But to finish on a more positive note I think this is a very interesting paper not because of the specific claims it makes but because of the way it tries to integrate ‘innate’ characteristic (physical attractiveness), personal proclivities and propensities (to commit crime), and the social world which influences and interacts with them. That is, it takes seriously the idea that people are individuals (and individual), and make personal choices about how and when to act – but their individuality and their choices (and possibility of choices) are structured and limited by the actions of others around them and the social and economic structures in which all are embedded. It seems to me that this kind of empirically robust ‘cross-level’ analysis is exactly the kind of thing we should be trying do more of in British criminology.
Wednesday, 17 March 2010
The hyper-criminalisation of normal human beahviour
The trouble with charts
Sunday, 14 March 2010
Campaign to reform UK libel laws
Police infiltration of anti-racist groups
Saturday, 13 March 2010
Sent down for a year for throwing a bottle
Thursday, 11 March 2010
How do they get away with it?
The level of disproportionality in police stop/search activity - and the public's acceptance of it - says, I think, as much about us as it does about the police. Despite all the advances in 'race' relations over the last 20-30 years I suspect there are many who think it's OK that Black and Asian people are 10 times more likely to be stopped than their White counterparts. That this is far far in excess of any differences in offending behaviour that can be identified doesn't seem to matter.
Indeed, the bias in police activity might even be one reason why some people appear so convinced that people from ethnic minorities are more involved in crime (something which, at the aggregate level, there's no evidence for whatsoever, although it may be true that some people from some minority groups are more involved in some types of crime than similar people from other groups - almost certainly because of variation in socio-economic conditions). A variation on the no smoke without fire argument, perhaps.
The police service itself, an organisation (genuinely, I think) interested in trying to develop better ways of dealing with increasing diversity and multi-culturalism, appears positively schizophrenic on this. Quite apart from the ethical and legal considerations, allowing this state of affairs to continue surely presents a massive barrier to police attempts to improve 'community relations', something which sits right in the middle of many current policies and strategies, from neighbourhood policing to Prevent.
Wednesday, 10 March 2010
Crime: up, down, shake it all around
Tuesday, 9 March 2010
The Venables affair
That said I'm not totally sure what to make of it all. For example, why was the information that Venables was back inside made public in the first place? There's already been so much written about the whole thing this seems to have gone down the memory hole - in fact, the government released the information the day before it was going to be published by the tabloids. Which obviously takes us right back to Steve Bell's cartoon. The tabloid press really does seem to run the government agenda sometimes.
Once the first 'drip' of information is out, of course, the pressure builds and builds until something has to give. But while I was initially thinking the government would have to reveal what he's (allegedly) done, I now think Jack Straw is doing the right thing by holding firm. Surely the clinching argument is that if there is to be a new trial it is imperative, for all concerned, that it should not be compromised by too much information being made public. I've heard several commentators make the point that celebrities (especially Gary Glitter) have had fair trials, even though events surrounding the accusations being so well-known. But this isn't really an argument, since presumably if the trials were fair this was despite the publicity, not because of it.
Until the facts are decided in a court of law, where's the public interest in airing the accusations in such an emotive case? I've yet to be convinced there is any.